Elsevier

Clinical Psychology Review

Volume 41, November 2015, Pages 3-15
Clinical Psychology Review

It's complicated: The relation between cognitive change procedures, cognitive change, and symptom change in cognitive therapy for depression

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2014.12.003Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Various therapeutic procedures produce both cognitive and symptom change.

  • Cognitive change appears to be a general mechanism of change.

  • Cognitive mediation studies often violate temporality assumptions.

  • A framework for research on cognitive mediation is proposed.

Abstract

Many attempts have been made to discover and characterize the mechanisms of change in psychotherapies for depression, yet no clear, evidence-based account of the relationship between therapeutic procedures, psychological mechanisms, and symptom improvement has emerged. Negatively-biased thinking plays an important role in the phenomenology of depression, and most theorists acknowledge that cognitive changes occur during successful treatments. However, the causal role of cognitive change procedures in promoting cognitive change and alleviating depressive symptoms has been questioned. We describe the methodological and inferential limitations of the relevant empirical investigations and provide recommendations for addressing them. We then develop a framework within which the possible links between cognitive procedures, cognitive change, and symptom change can be considered. We conclude that cognitive procedures are effective in alleviating symptoms of depression and that cognitive change, regardless of how it is achieved, contributes to symptom change, a pattern of findings that lends support to the cognitive theory of depression.

Introduction

It is widely accepted that psychological interventions can be effective in the treatment of Major Depressive Disorder (MDD). Despite a long history of attempts to understand the processes and mechanisms by which psychotherapies achieve their effects, consensus has not emerged regarding the associations between therapeutic procedures, psychological mechanisms, and the symptom improvement they lead to. Although basic psychological research supports the notion that maladaptive cognitions play an important role in the etiology and maintenance of depression (Ingram, Atchley, & Segal, 2011), some have questioned the role of cognitive change procedures in psychotherapies, as well as the status of cognitive change as a mechanism of symptom reduction. Interpreting the literature regarding cognitive change as a mediator of symptom change in cognitive therapy (CT), Kazdin (2007) concluded: “[W]e can state more confidently now than before that whatever may be the basis of changes with CT, it does not seem to be the cognitions as originally proposed.” (p. 08; see also Longmore & Worrell, 2007).

Proponents of a common factor view of psychotherapy (see Messer & Wampold, 2002), as well as advocates of the “third-wave” cognitive-behavioral therapies (e.g., Hayes, 2004), have also questioned the basic premises that underlie the cognitive therapies. A common factor account of the effectiveness of CT places little if any weight on the importance of specific techniques, cognitive or otherwise, and third-wave therapies were developed in part as a reaction against a mechanistic understanding of the relation of cognitive change to symptom change. In the context of these critiques, a clarification of the propositions implied by a cognitive theory of change in CT, as well as a review of the relevant evidence, is warranted. As has been recognized in other areas of health, an understanding of the mechanisms of therapeutic change can lead to better treatments.

Consistent with Kazdin (2009), we distinguish two terms, mechanism and mediator, that are used in descriptions and tests of models of therapeutic change. A mechanism is a phenomenon that, when changed by a treatment, causes change in symptoms. Two treatments could mobilize different mechanisms and yet lead to similar effects on symptoms (DeRubeis, Brotman, & Gibbons, 2005). In such cases, each of the treatments would be expected to produce greater change in its theory-relevant mechanism, relative to the other. However, because mechanisms are causal agents, when changes in a mechanism occur, they are followed by changes in symptoms, irrespective of the treatment in which the change occurred.

Investigations that address mechanistic theories often include tests of mediation. In studies of psychotherapeutic change, a mediator is a variable that accounts, statistically, for a treatment effect, either partially or fully. A study in which the statistical criteria for mediation are met can provide some support for a causal theory. However, because mediators are observed rather than manipulated, no test of mediation can rule out all competing causal accounts.

The theory behind CT can be stated thus: Engaging in procedures aimed at altering negatively biased beliefs and thinking styles leads to cognitive change, which is the mechanism by which depressive symptoms are reduced (Beck and Haigh, 2014, Beck et al., 1979). Evidence in support of cognitive change as a therapeutic mechanism would be that changes in cognitive processes or contents predict symptom change, irrespective of the cause of the cognitive change (Gelfand & DeRubeis, in press). However, it is possible that cognitive changes cause symptom change only, or especially, in the context of a treatment that focuses on changing cognitions. In this case, cognitive change would not be a general mechanism, but rather would act as a mediator that is specific to contexts in which cognitive procedures lead to the cognitive change.

Based on existing evidence (Barth et al., 2013, Cuijpers, Hollon, et al., 2013), it is safe to assert that cognitive change procedures, the defining features of CT, produce more symptom change, on average, than would occur simply with the passage of time. As tempting as it may be to infer that this provides strong support for a cognitive model of therapeutic change, such findings do not address the following questions (see Fig. 1):

  • 1.

    Do cognitive change procedures (X), which are the focus of CT, lead to greater reduction in depressive symptoms (Y) than do procedures that emphasize change in other domains, such as behaviors? (path c.)

  • 2.

    Do cognitive change procedures (X) produce greater cognitive change (M) than do non-cognitive change procedures (path a)?

  • 3.

    Do changes in cognition (M) lead to changes in depressive symptoms (Y), irrespective of the procedures that brought them about (path b)?

  • 4.

    Is cognitive change a specific mediator, such that changes in cognition resulting from cognitive change procedures (X*M) lead to greater symptom change (Y) than does cognitive change that is brought about by non-cognitive therapy procedures (path d)?

A study that would address all four of these questions would need to include: (a) a test of the relative effects on symptoms of treatment procedures that differ either in kind (e.g., cognitive vs. non-cognitive) or strength (e.g., high vs. low adherence to cognitive procedures) (differential efficacy of procedures on symptoms); (b) a test of the relative effects of different treatment procedures on cognitive change (differential effects of procedures on cognition); (c) a test of the effects of changes in cognition on symptom change (effects of cognitive change on symptom change); (d) a test of whether the relation of cognitions to symptoms differs as a function of the conditions under which cognitive change occurs (cognitive specificity). The validity of the inferences derived from this set of tests is limited, however, insofar as the research methods employed do not address reverse causality and third variable confounds.

In investigations of the effects of cognitive procedures on cognitive change and symptom change in depression, very few studies have included tests of all four of the relationships described above while also attempting to rule out reverse causality and third variable confounds (e. g., DeRubeis et al., 1990, Evans et al., 2013, Kuyken et al., 2010) and only one of these included a test of mediation (Kuyken et al.). Other investigations have included tests of all four relationships without including using methods that address these confounds (see Kazdin, 2007). Still other studies have been designed with a focus on a subset of the four relationships. In this paper we attempt to provide conceptual clarity and to address disagreements among commentators who have written about the cognitive mediation of psychotherapeutic change. We briefly discuss the ways in which negatively biased cognition has been implicated in depression and the different ways in which psychotherapies are purported to foster cognitive change. We then refer to recent reviews of the literature, relevant empirical studies that include tests of one or more of the four questions. We will distinguish between a variety of positions that have been proposed either implicitly or explicitly to account for relationships between cognitive change procedures, cognitive changes, and improvement in depression. We will assume that cognitive change can result from therapeutic interventions initiated by a therapist as well from other sources, such as positive life events. When we use the term cognitive change procedures we are referring to actions taken by the therapist with the aim of promoting cognitive change. In neither case do we assume that therapeutic techniques or psychological changes are uniquely cognitive or non-cognitive in nature. Rather, we adhere to conventional use of language to refer to cognitive change procedures as those that are primarily aimed at changing cognitions and cognitive change as change that is well-characterized as cognitive but may be associated with changes, such as behavioral change, that do not fit the “cognitive” rubric.

There is abundant evidence that negative thinking plays an important role in the phenomenology and ontology of depression. Measures of negative thinking have been shown to predict prospectively the onset, relapse, and recurrence of symptoms of depression (e.g., Mathews and MacLeod, 2005, Scher et al., 2005, Wenze et al., 2010). These and other findings suggest that negatively biased cognition is not merely characteristic of depression, but it also is a mechanism whereby depression arises and is maintained. A corollary is that changes in negative cognition can also be a mechanism by which symptoms of depression remit. A defining feature of the cognitive-behavioral therapies (CBTs), which includes CT, is the assumption that changes in thinking mediate the effects of therapeutic interventions on psychiatric symptoms (Dobson & Dozois, 2010). However, among different CBT approaches, as well as among clinical theorists who write about CBTs, there are differences regarding which aspects of cognition are presumed to be the most important targets of therapeutic intervention. Similarly, there is no broad consensus as to what are the most effective cognitive procedures for promoting cognitive change or symptom change.

A common set of distinctions within the general term cognition is between processes, contents, and compensatory skills. Information processing that is biased, for example, in directing attention to negative stimuli has been implicated in the pathology of depression (Ingram et al., 2011). Such biased information processing tendencies have been related to the concepts of schemas or modes (Beck, 1996). Although the use of refocusing and distancing is recommended in CT as a way of altering these processes (Beck, 2011), a major emphasis in treatment is in accumulating evidence that can be used to compare the validity of negative schemata with more accurate and adaptive alternatives. Proponents of third-wave CBTs have gone further and stress the value of shifting the focus of therapy away from the contents of cognitions and towards an exclusive focus on processes. As exemplified by the names of the most popular third-wave CBTs, mindfulness and acceptance techniques are emphasized in these treatments (Brown, Gaudiano, & Miller, 2011). In traditional CBTs, with their emphasis on the modification of beliefs and assumptions, an important distinction is between more immediate cognitive content and contents that remain latent until they are activated (e.g., schemas; Kwon & Oei, 1994). One further target of traditional CBTs is the development of skills that can be used to respond to depressogenic thoughts and negative affect (Barber & DeRubeis, 1989).

More than 25 years ago, Barber and DeRubeis (1989) noted the presence of “conceptual ambiguities regarding cognitive processes and contents as they have been applied in the cognitive therapy literature,” (p. 441). These ambiguities remain. For example, whereas a distinction has been drawn between cognitive processes and content (e.g., Nolen-Hoeksema, Wisco, & Lyubomirsky, 2008), there is considerable overlap in the referents and operationalizations of the constructs that are categorized as processes versus contents. For example, although a distinction between process and content is raised in discussions of the targets of third-wave CBTs, the study of maladaptive cognitive processes has focused on those processes that involve negative contents.

Although CBTs differ in the extent to which they target processes, content, and skills, a unifying assumption is that cognitive change mediates the effects of treatment on reduction or prevention of psychopathological symptoms. Thus, we will include investigations of changes in any of these three kinds of cognitive phenomena in our consideration of the links between cognitive change procedures, cognitive change, and symptom change.

Questions about the validity of a theory of psychotherapeutic change are best addressed by investigations of specific therapeutic procedures – such as the examination of evidence for and against a belief – instead of investigations of a therapeutic modality. This is because a treatment (say, CT) may contain multiple procedures, some of which may overlap with those that also define other psychotherapeutic modalities (Haaga, 2007). For example, descriptions of psychodynamic therapies often include techniques that could also be categorized as cognitive-behavioral (Barth, Michlig, & Munder, 2014). Moreover, practitioners of a therapy modality can, and in fact often do, use techniques from other modalities (McCarthy & Barber, 2009).

The effectiveness of cognitive change procedures as a means of bringing about symptom change has been questioned on the basis of findings from studies in which components of CBTs have been compared (e.g., Jacobson et al., 1996). Longmore and Worrell (2007, p. 174) ask whether “the direct, explicit modification of maladaptive cognitions [is] a necessary or sufficient intervention in CBT.” These authors then discussed studies that have failed to find a statistically significant difference between cognitive and behavioral interventions on short-term outcomes. They conclude from these data that cognitive change procedures do not contribute to symptom change beyond what is achieved via the non-cognitive procedures that are a part of CBT.

Longmore and Worrell (2007) infer from the lack of statistically significant differences in symptom change between cognitive and non-cognitive interventions that cognitive change is not a mechanism of symptom change. This interpretation of the kinds of findings reviewed by Longmore and Worrell, which is echoed in the writings of Hayes (2004; see also Jacobson et al., 1996), does not employ a distinction between therapeutic packages, therapeutic procedures, and mechanisms of therapeutic change. As observed by McCarthy and Barber (2009), a non-cognitive therapy session can include “cognitive” procedures such as comparing the evidence for and against a belief. More importantly, as Hofmann (2008) has argued, even when so-called non-cognitive procedures lead to symptom change, the mechanism of that change may nonetheless be cognitive. For example, assigning an activity, insofar as engagement in the activity contributes to mood change, could do so through the mechanism of a decrease in hopelessness. Thus, comparisons of treatment packages cannot provide the most informative tests of the relative efficacy of therapeutic procedures or of the mechanisms of symptom change. Instead, investigations of therapeutic modalities should include attempts to isolate the specific behaviors of the therapists – procedures or techniques – that lead to symptom change. To uncover the mechanism by which these procedures lead to outcome, such investigations should include measurements of a purported change variable.

Investigations of the processes of psychotherapeutic change are often focused on one of the three potential links between therapeutic procedures, psychological mechanisms, and outcomes. Mediation analyses can be used to characterize the network of relations among these variables (Baron and Kenny, 1986, MacKinnon and Fairchild, 2009). Tests of empirical data that meet the statistical criteria for mediation can contribute to our understanding of the causal links among measured constructs. Although other approaches, such as structural equation modeling (SEM), have also been recommended (Gelfand, Mensinger, & Tenhave, 2009), mediational analyses are the most widely used statistical tests of causal hypotheses. However, statistical tests alone, no matter how robust, cannot fully substantiate a set of causal claims. The validity of such claims rests on two assumptions that are difficult or impossible to meet.

The first assumption is that no causal variable has been omitted from the analysis (Gelfand et al., 2009). Even in the most thorough and rigorous test of mediation, it cannot be known if all causal variables have been included. Therefore, as there may be an unmeasured third variable that could represent the causal mechanism, the most that can be said when a positive finding is obtained in a mediation analysis is that the pattern in the data is consistent with a causal model. The second assumption is that the temporal order of the phenomena is consistent with the order implied by the tests (Gelfand et al., 2009). As has been noted by others (Haaga, 2007, Kazdin, 2007), it can be difficult to establish that changes in a putative mediator precede changes in the outcomes they are meant to explain. In fact, the temporal order assumption has rarely been met in tests of the mediation of the effects of treatments for MDD. Thus, an investigation in which temporal order is accounted for, even if it only provides a test of one link in the mediational chain, may provide stronger evidence in support of a causal hypothesis than one in which mediation testing is conducted but in which the temporal order assumption has not been met. Negative findings, on the other hand, can help to disconfirm causal hypotheses.

We describe empirical tests of each of the bivariate associations between measures of procedures, potential mediators, and outcomes, as well as investigations that have included mediation analyses. We will focus on the inferences supported by the findings, as well as on the ways in which common features of research designs place limits on these inferences.

Findings from randomized comparisons (i.e., outcome research) of treatment packages that focus on cognitive change (such as CT) versus treatment packages that do not (e.g., BA) provide evidence regarding the effects of cognitive procedures on symptom change. Another type of evidence that bears on the relation between procedures and outcomes comes from observational studies of courses of treatment that vary in the degree to which cognitive change procedures are employed (i.e., process-outcome research).

Tolin (2010), in his meta-analysis of outcome findings in anxiety or depressive disorders, found CBT to be superior to other treatments. Similarly, in the Vittengl, Clark, Dunn, and Jarrett (2007) meta-analysis, there was evidence that continuation CBT was superior in reducing relapse-recurrence rates after discontinuation, relative to other continuation therapies. When Barth et al. (2013) limited their meta-analytic review to trials that were adequately powered, their findings suggested that CBT, problem-solving (PST) and interpersonal therapies (IPT) for depression produce greater symptom reduction than do control conditions. By contrast, there was insufficient evidence for the efficacy of BA and social skills training, and the existing evidence did not suggest that psychodynamic and supportive therapies are more efficacious than control conditions. In a meta-analytic review of comparative outcome studies for patients with mild to moderate MDD, Cuijpers, van Straten, van Oppen, and Andersson (2008) concluded that the effects of CBTs do not differ from the effects observed in each of six other psychological treatments. This pattern of findings is consistent with the Dodo Bird verdict (Luborsky et al., 2002), a widely held belief that all psychotherapies are equally effective. Thus, the findings regarding the superiority of CBTs compared to other treatment modalities are equivocal or, at least, debatable, and likely hinge not only on the methods used to conduct the meta-analyses, but also on the focus (e.g., mild or moderate depression versus more severe depression) or the timeframe (acute vs. longer-term).

Within CBT for depression, variation in therapist adherence and competence has been found to account for variation in outcomes (Kuyken and Tsivrikos, 2008, Shaw et al., 1999). DeRubeis and Feeley (1990) found that, early in therapy, variation in therapists' use of concrete cognitive therapy techniques, such as examining evidence concerning belief, was related to subsequent symptom change. This finding has been replicated by Feeley, DeRubeis, and Gelfand (1999). Strunk, Brotman, and DeRubeis (2010) also replicated these findings, showing that the use of specific cognitive change methods but not behavioral methods, was related to subsequent change in depression.

Ilardi and Craighead (1994) argued that the specific cognitive procedures of CBTs cannot account for symptom change, on the assumption that most symptom change occurs before cognitive strategies are implemented. The findings referenced by Ilardi and Craighead were from trials in which CBT sessions were conducted biweekly for the first three to four weeks. Therefore, the change they were referring to had occurred by what in most cases was the sixth to eighth session. Since it has been shown that cognitive procedures are used extensively by cognitive therapists as early as the second session of therapy (e.g., DeRubeis and Feeley, 1990, Feeley et al., 1999, Strunk et al., 2010), there is nothing in the outcome literature that rules out the causal effects of cognitive procedures on symptom change. However, there are few investigations that have been designed to test the hypothesis that cognitive change procedures produce more symptom change than do non-cognitive change procedures.

Even if it were known that cognitive change procedures do not yield greater symptom improvement than other therapeutic interventions, this would not inform our understanding of the mechanisms through which CBTs, or other interventions, achieve their effects. In the approach to mediation analysis described by Baron and Kenny (1986), a significant effect of X (procedures) on Y (outcome) is required in order to proceed with the remaining tests. However, several authors have advocated approaches to mediation testing that do not require a between-treatment difference in outcomes (Kraemer et al., 2002, MacKinnon and Fairchild, 2009).

One of the intuitions that motivates these alternative approaches is that although equivalent outcomes between two treatments can be brought about by the same mechanism, alternatively, equivalently effective treatments may achieve their effects via different mechanisms (DeRubeis, Brotman, & Gibbons, 2005). For example, cognitive change procedures and procedures aimed at changing behavior may both ultimately work via the mechanism of cognitive change. However, it may instead be that they work via different mechanisms: cognitive change procedures via cognitive means and behavior change procedures via alternate means. In neither one of these cases would one proceed to conduct the tests recommended by Baron and Kenny (1986), although tests of mediation for each treatment would still be of interest. The presence in the research design of a condition that is less effective than either of the treatments, as well as the inclusion of variables that represent the different treatments' presumed mechanisms, would help to distinguish whether the comparable efficacy of the two treatments is driven by change in the same mechanism or by differential change in two or more different mechanisms.

The effect of cognitive change procedures on cognitive change can be examined by investigating changes in cognitive variables that occur during treatments that include cognitive change procedures. Hundt, Mignogna, Underhill, and Cully (2013) reviewed 13 studies in which the frequency of patients' use of CBT skills was assessed at the end of CBT treatments for depression, as well as 11 studies that focused on the quality of patients' CBT skills. In the majority of the investigations, increases in the frequency and quality of patients' use of cognitive restructuring and other CBT skills (e.g., thought records) were observed. Other reviews confirm significant cognitive changes following CBTs (Oei & Free, 1995). Process-outcome research provides another way of exploring the relation between cognitive procedures and cognitive change. In an observational study of CT processes, Webb et al. (2012) found that variation in therapists' use of concrete cognitive therapy techniques accounted for variability in patients' later use of cognitive strategies. Taken together, these findings suggest that cognitive change procedures lead to cognitive change.

Comparisons of antidepressant medication therapy versus CBTs have yielded evidence suggesting that cognitive procedures can result in greater change in depressive cognitions and cognitive processes, relative to medications (e.g., Dozois et al., 2009, Fresco et al., 2007, Ma and Teasdale, 2004, Segal et al., 1999, Segal et al., 2006). However, this pattern has not been consistently reported (DeRubeis et al., 1990, Fournier et al., 2013). Garratt, Ingram, Rand, and Sawalani (2007) argue that, in general, cognitive changes following medication treatments are more superficial whereas CBTs are able to facilitate deeper cognitive change, altering processes such as cognitive reactivity. In contrast to the comparisons of CBTs versus antidepressants, comparisons between CBTs and other psychotherapies have tended not to reveal differences in the magnitude of cognitive change (see Garratt et al.; Oei & Free, 1995).

Null findings regarding the magnitude of cognitive change following different treatment procedures may reflect the fact that equivalent cognitive change can follow from different therapeutic procedures. However, such findings could also reflect a misspecification of the cognitive change variable. The literature on the differential effects of CT and medications illustrates this point. Patients treated with antidepressants or CT typically show no difference in responses to the Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale (DAS; Weissman & Beck, 1978) when a standard administration of it is used. However, when the DAS has been given following a negative mood induction, patients who have gone through CT tend to endorse fewer negative cognitions than those who received medications (Segal et al., 1999, Segal et al., 2006). This kind of comparison, however, has not yet been reported between CBT and others psychotherapies.

Contemporaneous associations between cognitive change and symptom change during treatments for depression have been observed in numerous investigations (Garratt et al., 2007, Oei and Free, 1995). Within CBT, Christopher, Jacob, Neuhaus, Neary, and Fiola (2009) found that symptom improvement covaried with cognitive change, but not with behavioral change. Jacob, Christopher, and Neuhaus (2011), by contrast, found that both behavioral and cognitive changes were correlated with symptom improvement. In their review, Hundt et al. (2013) reported that in most of the studies of changes in cognitive and behavioral compensatory skills, significant positive relations between increases in these skills and symptom improvement were observed.

Findings of associations between cognitive change and symptom change might reflect a causal link from cognition to symptoms, but without a control for temporal confounds one cannot rule out the possibility that cognitions and symptoms change simultaneously, or that changes in symptoms lead to changes in cognition. However, in studies of cognitive change in CBT that have addressed the issue of temporal order, cognitive changes have been found to predict subsequent symptom change (e.g., Neimeyer and Feixas, 1990, Powers et al., 2008, Shirk et al., 2013). In CT, within-session cognitive changes predict “sudden gains” (Tang and DeRubeis, 1999a, Tang et al., 2005), which are associated with positive long-term outcomes (Aderka, Nickerson, Bøe, & Hofmann, 2012). Strunk, DeRubeis, Chiu, and Alvarez (2007) found that changes in patients' CT skills, as well their use of the skills, predicted resistance to relapse in the year following a course of CT for depression. Similarly, changes from pre- to post-treatment in extreme responding (Forand & DeRubeis, 2014), as well as a measure of cognitive reactivity at post-treatment (Segal et al., 1999, Teasdale et al., 2002), have been found to predict relapse.

Few studies have included tests of the mediation of the effects of cognitive procedures on symptom change. In Hundt et al.'s (2013) review of 22 studies that examined the relation between depression outcomes and a cognitive variable – the use of CBT skills – only 2 studies included statistical tests of mediation. In one of those (Gallagher-Thompson, Gray, Dupart, Jimenez, & Thompson, 2008), the superiority of a group CBT intervention relative to a control condition was statistically mediated by changes in CBT skills. However, cognitive change and symptom change were measured concurrently. Kuyken et al. (2010) randomized patients who had responded to medications to either continuation medication treatment or a mindfulness-based CT intervention (MBCT). Increases in self-compassion and in the use of mindfulness skills met criteria for mediation of the superior prevention effects of MBCT. Thus, both studies found evidence of the cognitive mediation of positive treatment effects. However, only Kuyken et al. employed a research design that could reasonably rule out reverse causality.

In a study that compared CBT, PST, and a waiting list condition, Warmerdam, van Straten, Jongsma, Twisk, and Cuijpers (2010) found that changes in dysfunctional attitudes, worry, and a negative problem orientation met the statistical criteria for the mediation of symptom change for both CBT and PST. Forman et al. (2012) found that changes in dysfunctional thinking and cognitive defusion mediated decreases in depressive symptoms following ACT and CT. However, like the Gallagher-Thompson et al. (2008) study, cognitive change and symptom change were measured concurrently in these studies.

Structural equation models (SEM) have also been employed to investigate the causal role of cognitive change in therapy. Burns and Spangler (2001) compared fit statistics across models that differed in the hypothesized direction of the relation between cognitive change and symptom change. The model that represented co-occurring cognitive change and symptom change fit the data better than one in which cognitive change preceded and predicted symptom change. Similar findings were obtained by Oei, Bullbeck, and Campbell (2006) in their SEM of data obtained during group CBT for depression. Although SEM has been recommended as an alternative to traditional tests for mediation (see Garratt et al., 2007, Haaga, 2007), if the temporal order in the model is not correctly specified, causal inferences are subject to the same limitation as are claims that follow from standard mediation models.

Virtually every published finding regarding the link between cognitive change and symptom change has been consistent with a cognitive model of change. Moreover, findings from tests of mediation suggest that cognitive changes can account for the effects produced by cognitive, as well as non-cognitive, change procedures. However few of the studies have included a control for temporal confounds, and the issue of temporality is a non-trivial one. This is exemplified in Smits, Rosenfield, McDonald, and Telch's (2006) study of CBT for social anxiety. Changes in one cognitive variable – the predicted probability of negative events – preceded and predicted change in symptoms, whereas changes in another cognitive variable – the predicted cost of a negative event – appeared to follow from symptom improvement. Thus, temporal confounds in the existing literature limit the confidence with which causal claims can be asserted. However, it does not follow that cognitive change does not cause symptom change, as Kazdin, 2007, Kazdin, 2009 has suggested. An assertion that cognitive change is not a mechanism of symptom change would gain support from null or negative findings obtained in studies with the necessary design features. To our knowledge, such evidence does not exist, and yet evidence of positive associations between cognitive change and symptom change are abundant. Moreover, positive evidence for alternative mechanisms of change is lacking. Thus, there is support for the hypothesis that cognitive change is a mechanism of symptom change. Although evidence consistent with this hypothesis comes largely from studies in which reverse causality has not been ruled out, there is little if any support for alternative causal accounts.

Tests of cognitive change as a mediator of symptom change specifically in the context of cognitive change procedures have been conducted by comparing associations between cognitive change and symptom change across different treatment conditions, especially CBTs vs. somatic therapies. DeRubeis et al. (1990) found that cognitive changes predicted subsequent symptom improvement for patients treated with CT, but not for those treated with antidepressant medications. They concluded that whereas in CT cognitive change may have driven further symptom change, the cognitive change observed in medication-treated patients may have been a result rather than a cause of symptom change. Quilty, McBride, and Bagby (2008) reached a similar conclusion from an investigation in which they used structural equation modeling to characterize pre- to post-treatment changes in cognitions and symptoms, although cognitive change and symptom change was measured concurrently. In a study of seasonal affective depression, Evans et al. (2013) reported that in CBT, but not in light therapy, cognitive changes during 6 weeks of treatment predicted depressive symptoms at 1-year winter follow-up. In contrast to the three studies just cited, each of which found specificity in the relation between cognitive change and symptom change, Segal and colleagues reported that cognitive reactivity predicted depressive relapse irrespective of whether it followed from treatment with CT or antidepressant medications (Segal et al., 1999, Segal et al., 2006).

The specificity of cognitive change has also been explored across psychotherapies. In a meta-analytic review of cognitive change and symptom change, Oei and Free (1995) reported a positive correlation between cognitive change and symptom change in studies that focused on non-CT psychotherapies, as well as in those that focused on CT. By contrast, the association of cognitive change and symptom change was small and non-significant in studies of antidepressant medication treatments. Similarly, Quilty et al. (2008) found that cognitive change co-varied with symptom change in both IPT and CBT. Warmerdam et al. (2010) also reported cognitive change as a mediator of the treatment effects of both PST and CBT relative to a waitlist condition. These findings suggest that cognitive change might be a non-specific mediator of change across PST, CBT, and IPT. However, none of these studies included controls for temporal confounds.

Forman, Herbert, Moitra, Yeomans, and Geller (2007) reported that theory-relevant variables were differentially associated with outcomes in CT versus Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT). In CT, but not in ACT, observing one's internal experience was associated with reduced levels of depression. By contrast, in ACT but not in CT, acceptance of internal experiences was associated with outcome. The authors replicated and extended these findings by showing that patients' use of cognitive and affective change strategies, as opposed to acceptance strategies, were related to positive outcomes in CT, whereas in ACT acceptance was positively related to outcome and changes in cognitive variables were negatively related to symptom change (Forman et al., 2012).

When a mediator is non-specific, it suggests that the phenomenon represented by the mediator may be a mechanism of therapeutic change (Gelfand & DeRubeis, in press). Findings of specific cognitive mediation raise the issue of why cognitive changes are related to outcomes in some treatment contexts but not in others. It is possible that the observed cognitive change variables in a given study do not reflect mechanisms in and of themselves. Rather, the variables may reflect phenomena that play a causal role if and only if other – unmeasured – phenomena are present. In the treatment in which cognitive mediation is observed, the assumption would be that the cognitive change variable and the variable that was not included combine to represent a single mechanism. Consistent with this idea, DeRubeis et al. (1990) hypothesized that in their CT condition, change in measured beliefs (e.g., DAS), combined with the acquisition and use of compensatory skills, which were not measured, accounted for symptom change in CT. Whereas medications did induce cognitive change, in that treatment the acquisition and use of compensatory skills was unlikely to have occurred to a similar degree, and therefore medications would not engage this mechanism. It is implied in any account of specific mediation that alternative causal paths are engaged by the different treatments.

Another hypothesis that is consistent with a finding of specific cognitive mediation is that changes in cognition are a proxy for a third variable only in the context of cognitive change procedures. For example, some have argued that a sufficient mechanism in treatment-induced change is the provision of a rationale specific to the treatment that allows patients to understand their symptoms (Frank & Frank, 1993). Under this view, it is not cognitive change per se that leads to symptom change. Instead, cognitive change in a cognitive treatment is a proxy for patients' belief in the treatment rationale, which is itself the mechanism of change. What follows from this kind of analysis is that cognitive change in the context of a cognitive treatment will lead to symptom improvement, as will bringing about behavior change in the context of a behavioral treatment, changing role conflicts in IPT, and so forth.

In Table 1 we outline a framework for representing competing views of the links between cognitive change procedures, cognitive change, and symptom change. Regarding the differential effects of procedures on symptoms (path c in Fig. 1), a cognitive change procedure may be: more efficacious than (+ 1), equally efficacious as (0), or less efficacious than (− 1) a non-cognitive change procedure in the treatment of depression. Considering the differential effects of procedures on cognition (path a), a cognitive change procedure may alter cognitions: more than (+ 1), equal to (0), or less than (− 1) a non-cognitive change procedure. Regarding the effect of cognitive change on symptom change (path b), there may either be a causal effect (+ 1) or no causal effect (0). Finally, regarding cognitive specificity (path d), cognitive change might act as a non-specific mediator (0), irrespective of how the change occurred, or the magnitude of the effects of cognitive change on symptom change may depend upon whether cognitive change procedures brought about the cognitive change (+ 1).

A comprehensive theory of therapeutic change implies a set of claims or hypotheses that integrates the four links. Our framework, using the notation just described, yields 36 possible models (see Appendix A) that can be distinguished primarily by whether they consider that cognitive changes are: (1) mechanisms of change, (2) not mechanisms of change, or (3) specific mediators of therapeutic effects. The relative efficacy of cognitive and non-cognitive procedures in producing symptom change further distinguishes the positions. However, a relatively small subset of the models generated by this framework warrant detailed consideration. For example, models in which cognitive change does not mediate symptom change in any way can be discussed as representing one position; in these models the magnitude of cognitive change following different therapeutic procedures becomes relatively unimportant (X, X, 0, 0). Other models are illogical or internally inconsistent (e.g., a model of cognitive change as a general mechanism that posits that cognitive change procedures produce superior symptom change but inferior cognitive change, + 1, − 1, + 1, 0). Finally, we do not discuss models in which cognitive change is both a specific and a non-specific mediator of symptom change, although we recognize that one or more of the models with this feature might be worthy of consideration.

We are left with five kinds of models that are consistent with theoretical positions outlined or suggested by experts in the literature on psychotherapy (see Table 2). In the following, we focus on the relevant empirical evidence that speaks to the links, and the connections among them, implied by each of these five positions. As in the reviews of evidence regarding the links, we consider issues of omitted variables and temporal confounds. We also describe research that would fill gaps in the empirical literature. We begin with three positions that, although different from one another in an important respect, each assume that change in cognitions plays a causal role in symptom change (i.e., cognitive change is a mechanism). A fourth position assumes that there is no causal link between cognition change and symptom change (i.e., no cognitive mediation). The last position we consider is one that ascribes a mediational role to cognitive change, but only in the context of cognitive change procedures (i.e., specific cognitive mediation).

The three positions outlined below share the view that cognitive change is a therapeutic mechanism, but they differ in regard to whether cognitive procedures are more, or less, effective than other procedures in bringing about cognitive change and, as a consequence, symptom change.

A strong proposition regarding the efficacy of cognitive change procedures is that: (a) such procedures are more efficacious than alternative change procedures because (b) they are the most powerful means of producing cognitive change and (c) changing cognitions results in symptom change (or resistance to relapse). Evidence for this position is that CBTs, which emphasize cognitive change procedures, yield better outcomes than medications over the long term (Cuijpers, Hollon, et al., 2013), and that changes in cognitive reactivity are greater in CT relative to medications (Segal et al., 1999, Segal et al., 2006). Whereas the relative efficacy and the mediation of the long-term effects of CBTs in relation to antidepressants are clear, less is known about the relative long-term effects of CBTs compared to other psychotherapies.

A moderate proposition regarding the efficacy of cognitive change procedures is that: (a) they are equally as effective as alternative change procedures because (b) they are among the procedures that lead to cognitive change and (c) cognitive change causes symptom change. Hofmann (2008) suggests that non-cognitive change procedures, such as exposure and behavioral activation, as well as cognitive change procedures, may work through cognitive change to produce symptom change. Similarly, Warmerdam et al. (2010) suggest that changes in cognitive measures are a proxy for common cognitive processes that account for change across psychotherapies for depression. Evidence for this position is that CBT has been found to yield short-term outcomes (Cuijpers et al., 2008) and changes in cognitive measures (Oei & Free, 1995) that are similar to those observed in other psychotherapies, and that changes in cognitions co-vary with changes in symptoms across treatments.

A third proposition regarding the efficacy of cognitive change procedures is that: (a) they are less effective than alternative change procedures because (b) cognitive change procedures are a relatively ineffective means of producing the cognitive changes that (c) result in symptom change. Some suggest that the cognitive change procedures in CBTs detract from therapeutic activities that would lead more efficiently to symptom change, such as behavioral activation (Jacobson et al., 1996). Others go further and argue that cognitive change procedures can produce harm in that they encourage a maladaptive focus on cognitive processes, through the process of examining beliefs, or they invalidate the patient (Linehan, 1993). As Linehan stated, “[s]tandard behavior therapy (including standard cognitive-behavioral therapy) by itself, at least as I practiced it, invalidated my patients” (p. 77). To our knowledge there is no evidence that cognitive procedures are less effective than alternatives in producing cognitive change.

It is possible, as Kazdin (2007) has asserted, that cognitive mechanisms do not account for the efficacy of CBT for depression. His conclusion is based on the fact that although numerous studies have revealed links between cognitive change and symptom change during treatment, only a handful of studies have used designs that could address reverse causality or third variable explanations. Given that Kazdin's claim rests on the acceptance of a null hypothesis, evidence is required that a non-cognitive mechanism, such as behavioral change, is responsible for symptom change in CBT, whereas cognitive change is not. We are aware of no such evidence.

Aside from CBT, the most extensively-researched psychotherapies for depression are IPT and PST (Barth et al., 2013), making the mechanisms implied by their respective theories of change likely candidates as non-cognitive mechanisms of psychotherapeutic change. However, there has been little research on the mechanisms of change in IPT (Markowitz & Weissman, 2004) and PST (Mynors-Wallis, 2002), and the roles of changes in interpersonal processes or problem-solving skills have received little attention in research on CT for depression. Thus, to bolster the claim that cognitive change is not a mechanism of symptom change in CT, findings would need to emerge that reveal that non-cognitive constructs account for symptom change over and above the effects of cognitive change.

Finally, it is possible that cognitive change functions as a mediator of symptom change only when it occurs in the context of a treatment that emphasizes cognitive change procedures. Although there is evidence that cognitive change is associated with symptom change in CBT, IPT, and PST, there is also some evidence that this association is not found in somatic therapies (i.e., light therapy and antidepressants; DeRubeis et al., 1990, Evans et al., 2013, Quilty et al., 2008) or in ACT (Forman et al., 2007, Forman et al., 2012). Thus, the position that cognitive change is a specific mediator has some support. As a way of integrating these seemingly disparate findings, it may be that the measurement of self-reported cognitive change in CBTs captures change in cognitive vulnerabilities whereas in treatment with somatic therapies, measurements of cognitive change capture the deactivation of state-dependent negative cognitions. (For a similar argument, see Evans et al., 2013). This hypothesis is endorsed by Garratt et al. (2007), who conclude that “evidence suggests that cognitive changes associated with pharmacotherapy are more superficial than those associated with cognitive therapy” (p. 224). Alternatively, cognitive change in a CBT may be a proxy for a third variable, such as adopting the treatment rationale.

In the context of evidence that cognitive change is a mediator of change in IPT and PST as well as in CBT, it is tempting to propose that most or all psychotherapies work via cognitive change whereas somatic therapies work via other mechanisms. However, the findings of Forman et al. (2007, 2012), in their comparisons of CT and ACT, suggest the possibility that at least these two psychotherapies work via different mechanisms. Insofar as PST and IPT may engage the same mechanisms as CBT, they too might differ from ACT, but thus far there are no data that speak to this possibility. Interestingly, although MBCT has also been classified as a third wave CBT, in studies of MBCT cognitive change also correlates with symptom change. For example, Manicavasagar, Perich, and Parker (2012) reported that decreases in rumination correlated with change both in MBCT and CBT.

Our review suggests that the evidence available to address questions concerning cognitive mediation is limited in a variety of ways. The position with the most consistent empirical support is that cognitive change is a mechanism of change in therapies for depression and that cognitive as well as non-cognitive change procedures can produce similar levels of cognitive change and therefore similar levels of symptom improvement (position 1B).

CBT is the most extensively researched form of psychotherapy for depression. Findings from dismantling and component studies, which are often cited as evidence against a cognitive model of change, can only address claims that cognitive procedures are not necessary for the induction of cognitive change, or not superior to the non-cognitive procedures in a therapy package. Across courses of CT or CBT, the contents and processes of patients' thinking undergo change. They evidence increases in meta-cognitive awareness and compensatory skills, allowing them to respond to their biased or maladaptive negative thoughts. Insofar as these changes are also brought about by therapeutic procedures that do not expressly address cognitive contents or processes, this is often taken as evidence against the cognitive theory of depression, yet in fact it follows logically from a cognitive theory. If cognitive changes are a mechanism by which depressive symptoms are reduced (or relapses are prevented), cognitive change should predict symptom change (or relapse prevention) irrespective of how cognitive change is brought about. There are suggestions in the literature that somatic therapies as well as some psychotherapies that focus on internal experiences (e.g., ACT) may work through mechanisms that are not well-characterized as cognitive. Any cognitive change that occurs in these therapies may thus be a byproduct of the activation of other mechanisms. This would be consistent with the idea that the relation between cognition and affect is reciprocal (Mayberg et al., 1999).

Section snippets

Attention to temporal features

It is understood that the best evidence regarding the mediational status of a phenomenon, vis a vis the effects of procedures on subsequent symptom status, is obtained in studies in which the temporal order (procedures  mediator  outcome) can be established unambiguously. We have highlighted how, in empirical tests, the failure to attend to the temporal features of a mediation model severely limits the causal inferences that can be supported by the data. Investigators have called for the early

Summary and conclusions

Our review of the many methodological and inferential issues that arise when studying cognitive mediation of cognitive procedures highlights how the study of mechanisms of change in treatment is a difficult enterprise. The evidence for bivariate associations between cognitive procedures and symptom change, cognitive procedures and cognitive change, and cognitive change and symptom change is clear. Findings from experimental research suggest that strategies that involve cognitive reappraisal,

References (98)

  • R.A. Neimeyer et al.

    The role of homework and skill acquisition in the outcome of group cognitive therapy for depression

    Behavior Therapy

    (1990)
  • T.P.S. Oei et al.

    Cognitive change process during group cognitive behaviour therapy for depression

    Journal of Affective Disorders

    (2006)
  • D.V. Powers et al.

    The benefits of using psychotherapy skills following treatment for depression: An examination of “afterwork” and a test of the skills hypothesis in older adults

    Cognitive and Behavioral Practice

    (2008)
  • C.D. Scher et al.

    Cognitive reactivity and vulnerability: Empirical evaluation of construct activation and cognitive diatheses in unipolar depression

    Clinical Psychology Review

    (2005)
  • D.R. Strunk et al.

    The process of change in cognitive therapy for depression: Predictors of early inter-session symptom gains

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2010)
  • D.F. Tolin

    Is cognitive-behavioral therapy more effective than other therapies?: A meta-analytic review

    Clinical Psychology Review

    (2010)
  • L. Warmerdam et al.

    Online cognitive behavioral therapy and problem-solving therapy for depressive symptoms: Exploring mechanisms of change

    Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry

    (2010)
  • I.M. Aderka et al.

    Sudden gains during psychological treatments of anxiety and depression: A meta-analysis

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (2012)
  • J.P. Barber et al.

    On second thought: Where the action is in cognitive therapy for depression

    Cognitive Therapy and Research

    (1989)
  • J.P. Barber et al.

    The ways of responding: A scale to assess compensatory skills taught in cognitive therapy

    Behavioral Assessment

    (1992)
  • R.M. Baron et al.

    The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1986)
  • J. Barth et al.

    Unique and shared techniques in cognitive-behavioural and short-term psychodynamic psychotherapy: A content analysis of randomised trials on depression

    Health Psychology and Behavioral Medicine: an Open Access Journal

    (2014)
  • J. Barth et al.

    Comparative efficacy of seven psychotherapeutic interventions for patients with depression: A network meta-analysis

    PLoS Medicine

    (2013)
  • A.T. Beck

    Beyond belief: A theory of modes, personality, and psychopathology

  • A.T. Beck et al.

    Advances in cognitive theory and therapy: The generic cognitive model

    Annual Review of Clinical Psychology

    (2014)
  • A.T. Beck et al.

    Cognitive therapy of depression

    (1979)
  • J.S. Beck

    Cognitive behavior therapy: Basics and beyond

    (2011)
  • E.C. Bell et al.

    Are the parts as good as the whole? A meta-analysis of component treatment studies

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (2013)
  • L.A. Brown et al.

    Investigating the similarities and differences between practitioners of second-and third-wave cognitive-behavioral therapies

    Behavior Modification

    (2011)
  • M.S. Christopher et al.

    Cognitive and behavioral changes related to symptom improvement among patients with a mood disorder receiving intensive cognitive-behavioral therapy

    Journal of Psychiatric Practice

    (2009)
  • P. Cuijpers et al.

    Does cognitive behaviour therapy have an enduring effect that is superior to keeping patients on continuation pharmacotherapy? A meta-analysis

    BMJ Open

    (2013)
  • P. Cuijpers et al.

    The efficacy of psychotherapy and pharmacotherapy in treating depressive and anxiety disorders: A meta‐analysis of direct comparisons

    World Psychiatry

    (2013)
  • P. Cuijpers et al.

    Psychotherapy for depression in adults: A meta-analysis of comparative outcome studies

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (2008)
  • P. Cuijpers et al.

    Are psychological and pharmacologic interventions equally effective in the treatment of adult depressive disorders? A meta-analysis of comparative studies

    Journal of Clinical Psychiatry

    (2008)
  • R.J. DeRubeis et al.

    A conceptual and methodological analysis of the nonspecifics argument

    Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice

    (2005)
  • R.J. DeRubeis et al.

    How does cognitive therapy work? Cognitive change and symptom change in cognitive therapy and pharmacotherapy for depression

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (1990)
  • R.J. DeRubeis et al.

    Determinants of change in cognitive therapy for depression

    Cognitive Therapy and Research

    (1990)
  • R.J. DeRubeis et al.

    Understanding processes of change: How some patients reveal more than others – and some groups of therapists less – about what matters in psychotherapy

    Psychotherapy Research

    (2014)
  • R.J. DeRubeis et al.

    Cognitive therapy vs medications in the treatment of moderate to severe depression

    Archives of General Psychiatry

    (2005)
  • K.S. Dobson et al.

    Historical and philosophical bases of the cognitive-behavioral therapies

  • D.J. Dozois et al.

    Changes in self-schema structure in cognitive therapy for major depressive disorder: A randomized clinical trial

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (2009)
  • M. Evans et al.

    Cognitive change across cognitive-behavioral and light therapy treatments for seasonal affective disorder: What accounts for clinical status the next winter?

    Cognitive Therapy and Research

    (2013)
  • M. Feeley et al.

    The temporal relation of adherence and alliance to symptom change in cognitive therapy for depression

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (1999)
  • C. Firk et al.

    Serotonin transporter genotype modulates cognitive reappraisal of negative emotions: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study

    Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience

    (2013)
  • N.R. Forand et al.

    Extreme response style and symptom return after depression treatment: The role of positive extreme responding

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (2014)
  • E.M. Forman et al.

    A randomized controlled effectiveness trial of acceptance and commitment therapy and cognitive therapy for anxiety and depression

    Behavior Modification

    (2007)
  • J.C. Fournier et al.

    Psychotherapy and Neuroimaging

    FOCUS: The Journal of Lifelong Learning in Psychiatry

    (2014)
  • J.D. Frank et al.

    Persuasion and healing: A comparative study of psychotherapy

    (1993)
  • D.M. Fresco et al.

    Relationship of posttreatment decentering and cognitive reactivity to relapse in major depression

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (2007)
  • Cited by (148)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text